[新知] ScienceDaily-一朝被蛇咬十年怕草繩

看板Cognitive作者 (mulkcs)時間14年前 (2010/07/27 12:58), 編輯推噓2(2010)
留言12則, 2人參與, 最新討論串1/1
Once Bitten, Twice Shy: Temperature Switch Triggers Aversive Memory ScienceDaily (July 26, 2010) — It is common sense that it's worth learning what things and situations are harmful to us if we want to have a long and healthy life. For example, after getting a nasty sunburn, we learn our lesson, and apply sun cream before going sunbathing next time. The importance of such learned avoidance strategies is reflected by the fact that even fruit flies possess them. These tiny flies can learn to associate a particular odor with a mild electrical shock. Once they learned this association, they steer clear of the source of that particular odor in future. Scientists at the Max Planck Institute of Neurobiology now succeeded in identifying three nerve cells that play a role in the formation of this complex association. By altering the surrounding temperature, they could selectively switch certain cells on and off while the insects moved about freely and learned. Prevention is better than cure and avoidance strategies often help to save us from adversity. A child, for example, quickly learns not to touch a hot stove once it burned its fingers. Avoidance behavior is so essential that even the comparatively simple brain of the fruit fly excels in it. If, for example, a fruit fly is presented with a certain odor together with an electric shock, it quickly learns to avoid this particular odor by moving or flying off in the opposite direction. Yet, what actually happens in the brain when two such different stimuli as an odor and an electric shock are linked up with each other to cause a change in behavior? It was precisely this basic phenomenon that scientists at the Max Planck Institute for Neurobiology were determined to track down. The advantages of the fruit fly The Max Planck Research Group "Behavioral Genetics," led by Hiromu Tanimoto, investigates what happens in the brain of the fruit fly when it learns to avoid something. Given that the fruit fly's brain is nothing short of minute, one tends to wonder why the scientists investigate this phenomenon in the fruit fly. There are two good reasons for their choice. First of all, the brain of this insect is composed of about one hundred thousand nerve cells and is therefore considerably more straightforward than, say, a human brain which has about one hundred billion nerve cells. The cells responsible for avoidance behavior in the fruit fly can therefore be identified much more readily. What is more, the researchers can use the wide range of genetic tools that is already available for the fruit fly to activate or deactivate certain functions of the animal's brain with great precision. "We used precisely these qualities to our own advantage," Hiromu Tanimoto explains. The scientists already knew that the connection between an odor and an electric shock occurs inside the mushroom-body -- a structure in the brain of the fly that consists of some 2000 nerve cells. It is also clear that the neurotransmitter dopamine enables the flies to learn to associate a potential source of danger with a certain odor. However, until now, it was unclear as to which of the dopaminergic cells are actually responsible for this. Non-invasive manipulation "The problem was that we had to determine which of the nerve cells release dopamine while the flies are moving about and learning to avoid the odor," Tanimoto recapitulates on the study. This is precisely what the scientists have now succeeded in doing. They introduced a "temperature selector" into dopamine-releasing cells that contact cells of the mushroom-body. This stowaway gene stimulated the nerve cells in one set of flies to release dopamine as soon as the room temperature increased slightly. In a second set of flies, a different gene caused the nerve cells to become deactivated once room temperature was increased, no matter what stimulus Tanimoto and his staff presented to the insects. Thus equipped to manipulate, the scientists could demonstrate that the activity of three dopamine-releasing cells is essential for a detected odor to be associated with a negative experience. The decisive role of these three cells was unambiguous: if the cells were temperature-activated while the flies picked up on an odor, then the insects learned to avoid the odor -- even without the electric shock that constituted the negative association with the odor. "We can now actually examine the function of individual nerve cells in an active and behaving animal. This opens up new horizons," Yoshinori Aso, who is visibly delighted with the outcome of his experiment, adds. Step by step, the scientists now plan to get to the bottom of how experiences are linked to each other and how behavior modification develops. -- 原始網址:(有圖) http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2010/07/100726101201.htm -- ※ 發信站: 批踢踢實業坊(ptt.cc) ◆ From: 140.112.4.195

07/31 18:46, , 1F
比較好奇的是,後天學習和先天本能在這個Model中是否能區分
07/31 18:46, 1F

08/01 00:40, , 2F
他這裡做的應該比較像是後天學習...
08/01 00:40, 2F

08/01 00:40, , 3F
還是你說得趨吉避凶是先天本能?
08/01 00:40, 3F

08/01 00:40, , 4F
^是指
08/01 00:40, 4F

08/02 13:57, , 5F
我主要是被這句話給迷惑:if the cells were temperature-
08/02 13:57, 5F

08/02 13:58, , 6F
activated while the flies picked up on an odor, then the
08/02 13:58, 6F

08/02 13:58, , 7F
insects learned to avoid the odor -- even without the
08/02 13:58, 7F

08/02 13:58, , 8F
electric shock
08/02 13:58, 8F

08/02 14:00, , 9F
一種可能性是 shock 訊息的傳導路徑被找到,人為操弄模擬了
08/02 14:00, 9F

08/02 14:00, , 10F
電擊;另外一個可能性是這條路徑是天生的,只要刺激它就會產
08/02 14:00, 10F

08/02 14:01, , 11F
生趨避效果。
08/02 14:01, 11F

08/04 23:57, , 12F
我看文章感覺是後者。
08/04 23:57, 12F
文章代碼(AID): #1CJcTUb8 (Cognitive)